SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 197
Hypothesis Testing
The logic of statistical hypothesis testing follows the 
logic of judicial decision making.
A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty 
or not guilty.
A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty 
or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous 
decision, guilty or 
not guilty.
A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty 
or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous 
decision, guilty or 
not guilty. There is no in-between 
or partial decision.
A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty 
or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous 
decision, guilty or 
not guilty. There is no in-between 
or partial decision. 
The jury does not begin its 
decision-making process in 
a neutral position.
A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty 
or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous 
decision, guilty or 
not guilty. There is no in-between 
or partial decision. 
The jury does not begin its 
decision-making process in 
a neutral position. 
The default position is “not guilty.”
A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty 
or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous 
decision, guilty or 
not guilty. There is no in-between 
or partial decision. 
The jury does not begin its 
decision-making process in 
a neutral position. 
The default position is “not guilty.” 
The prosecution must mount enough evidence to 
convince the jury to move from its default position of 
not guilty to a verdict of guilty.
The jury will make a decision which may or may not 
coincide with reality.
When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
is, in reality, not guilty, 
It is true because the not guilty (negative) decision 
aligns with the not guilty (negative) reality.
When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
is, in reality, not guilty, they have made a correct 
decision called a “true negative decision.” 
It is true because the not guilty (negative) decision 
aligns with the not guilty (negative) reality.
When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
is, in reality, not guilty, they have made a correct 
decision called a “true negative decision.” 
It is true because the not guilty (negative) decision 
aligns with the not guilty (negative) reality. 
not guilty 
and I really 
wasn’t guilty! 
true negative
When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in 
reality, guilty, 
It is true because the guilty (positive) decision aligns 
with the guilty (positive) reality.
When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in 
reality, guilty, they have made a correct decision called 
a “true positive” decision. 
It is true because the guilty (positive) decision aligns 
with the guilty (positive) reality.
When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in 
reality, guilty, they have made a correct decision called 
a “true positive” decision. 
It is true because the guilty (positive) decision aligns 
with the guilty (positive) reality. 
guilty 
and I really 
WAS guilty! 
true positive
When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
is, in reality, guilty,
When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
is, in reality, guilty, they have made an incorrect 
decision called a “false negative error” which is also 
called a Type II or beta error.
When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
is, in reality, guilty, they have made an incorrect 
decision called a “false negative error” which is also 
called a Type II or beta error. 
It is false because the “not guilty” (negative) decision 
does not align with the guilty (positive) reality. 
not guilty 
and I really 
WAS guilty! 
false negative
When a jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in 
reality, not guilty,
When a jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in 
reality, not guilty, they have made an incorrect decision 
called a “false positive error”
When a jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in 
reality, not guilty, they have made an incorrect decision 
called a “false positive error” which is also called a Type 
I or alpha error.
When a jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in 
reality, not guilty, they have made an incorrect decision 
called a “false positive error” which is also called a Type 
I or alpha error. 
It is false because the “guilty” (positive) decision is not 
aligned with the not guilty (negative) reality. 
guilty 
but I really 
WASN’T guilty! 
false positive
Although we prefer correct decisions, if we cannot be 
correct, we prefer the false negative error over the false 
positive error. 
In other words you’d rather render a “NOT GUILTY” 
verdict when there is GUILT. 
Than a “GUILTY” verdict where there is NO GUILT.
Although we prefer correct decisions, if we cannot be 
correct, we prefer the false negative error over the false 
positive error. 
In other words you’d rather render a “NOT GUILTY” 
verdict when there is GUILT. 
not guilty 
and I really 
WAS guilty! 
Than a “GUILTY” verdict where there is NO GUILT.
Although we prefer correct decisions, if we cannot be 
correct, we prefer the false negative error over the false 
positive error. 
In other words you’d rather render a “NOT GUILTY” 
verdict when there is GUILT. 
not guilty 
and I really 
WAS guilty! 
Than a “GUILTY” verdict where there is NO GUILT. 
guilty 
but I really 
WASN’T guilty!
In judicial decisions we would rather let a guilty 
defendant go free . . . 
than convict and imprison an innocent defendant. 
Our default position of “not guilty” supports this 
preference and protects against the least favorable 
condition.
In judicial decisions we would rather let a guilty 
defendant go free . . . 
than convict and imprison an innocent defendant. 
Our default position of “not guilty” supports this 
preference and protects against the least favorable 
condition.
In judicial decisions we would rather let a guilty 
defendant go free . . . 
than convict and imprison an innocent defendant. 
Our default position of “not guilty” supports this 
preference and protects against the least favorable 
condition.
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow:
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow: 
What type of 
decision is 
made when 
a guilty (+) 
verdict is 
rendered and 
the person is 
guilty (+)?
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow: 
What type of 
decision is 
made when 
a guilty (+) 
verdict is 
rendered and 
the person is 
guilty (+)?
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow: 
What type of 
decision is 
made when 
a not guilty (-) 
verdict is 
rendered and 
the person is 
not guilty (-)?
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow: 
What type of 
decision is 
made when 
a not guilty (-) 
verdict is 
rendered and 
the person is 
not guilty (-)?
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow: 
What type of 
decision is 
made when 
a guilty (+) 
verdict is 
rendered and 
the person is 
not guilty (-)?
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow: 
What type of 
decision is 
made when 
a guilty (+) 
verdict is 
rendered and 
the person is 
not guilty (-)?
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow: 
What type of 
decision is 
made when 
a not guilty (-) 
verdict is 
rendered and 
the person is 
guilty (+) ?
Review the following slide and answer the questions 
that follow: 
What type of 
decision is 
made when 
a not guilty (-) 
verdict is 
rendered and 
the person is 
guilty (+) ?
Each conviction protects against Type I error at a 
different stringency according to the gravity of the 
punishment to be imposed.
The haunting reality is that we really never know the 
reality of the guilt or innocence of defendants. 
We make our best decisions knowing that there is a 
probability that we have made an error.
The haunting reality is that we really never know the 
reality of the guilt or innocence of defendants. 
We make our best decisions knowing that there is a 
probability that we have made an error.
Statistical hypothesis testing and decision-making are 
directly analogous to judicial decision making. 
Judicial Decisions 
Statistical Decisions
Let’s consider an example: 
A statistician is asked to decide whether a difference 
exists between two groups of people in terms of some 
attribute (e.g., excitability). 
It is a dichotomous decision (meaning only two 
options), different or not different. 
There is no in-between or partial decision.
Let’s consider an example: 
A statistician is asked to decide whether a difference 
exists between two groups of people in terms of some 
attribute (e.g., excitability). 
It is a dichotomous decision (meaning only two 
options), different or not different. 
There is no in-between or partial decision.
Let’s consider an example: 
A statistician is asked to decide whether a difference 
exists between two groups of people in terms of some 
attribute (e.g., excitability). 
It is a dichotomous decision (meaning only two 
options), different or not different. 
There is no in-between or partial decision. x
The statistician does not begin her decision-making in a 
neutral position. 
The default position is “not different.” 
This is also called the “null hypothesis.”
The statistician does not begin her decision-making in a 
neutral position. 
The default position is “not different.” 
This is also called the “null hypothesis.”
The statistician does not begin her decision-making in a 
neutral position. 
The default position is “not different.” 
This is also called the “null hypothesis.”
The research findings must present sufficient evidence 
to convince the statistician to move from her default 
position of no difference to a conclusion that the 
groups are different in terms of the attribute.
The statistician will make a decision which may or may 
not coincide with reality. 
The apparent differences may be due to chance or may 
be real.
The statistician will make a decision which may or may 
not coincide with reality. 
The apparent differences may be due to chance or may 
be real. 
OR Something 
that is really 
happening
When the statistician decides “not different” (fails to 
reject the null hypothesis, maintains the default 
position) and the groups are, in reality, not different, 
she has made a correct decision called a “true negative 
decision.” 
true negative
It is true because the “no difference” (negative) 
decision aligns with “no difference” reality. 
not guilty 
and I really 
WASN’T guilty! 
true negative
When the statistician decides that there is a difference 
(rejects the null hypothesis, moves off of the default 
position) and the groups are, in reality, different, she 
has made a correct decision called a true positive 
decision. 
true positive
It is true because the “different” (positive) decision 
aligns with the “different” (positive) reality. 
guilty 
and I really 
WAS guilty! 
true positive
When the statistician decides “not different” (fails to 
reject the null hypothesis, maintains the default 
position) and the group are, in reality different, she has 
made a false negative error. 
false negative
It is false because the decision of no difference 
(negative) does not align with difference (positive) 
reality. 
not guilty 
false negative 
Ha ha! and I 
really 
WAS guilty!
Although we prefer correct decisions, if we cannot be 
correct, we then prefer false negative error over the 
alternative error.
When a statistician decides that there is a difference 
(positive) between the groups and rejects the null 
hypothesis of no difference and, in reality, there is no 
difference, she has made a false positive error (also 
called Type I error or alpha error.) 
false positive
It is false because the “difference” (positive) decision 
does not align with the “no difference” (negative) 
reality. 
guilty 
but I really 
WASN’T guilty! 
false positive
Our hypothesis testing conventions protect against 
false positive, Type I error by holding a default position 
of the null hypothesis. 
α 
Beware of 
Type I Error
We set a standard of evidence that is required before 
rejecting the default null hypothesis.
The standard of evidence is based on the probability 
density of the sampling distribution.
Using probability density we can estimate the 
probability of Type I error. 
If the mean of the 
sample is here, then we 
have a .0001 or .01% 
chance that we made a 
Type I error.
Or in other words, we have a 
.01% chance of rejecting the 
null hypothesis that the 
group scores come from two 
different populations 
(claiming guilty) and being 
wrong when both groups 
were really part of the same 
population (not guilty)
When the probability of Type I error is at a low enough 
level, we reject the default, null hypothesis. 
Like in our previous example.
The conventional level of tolerable Type I error is .05. 
95% 
.05 or 5% chance that 
we selected a sample 
from this population 
and claimed it was a 
sample from another 
population = false 
positive
This means that out of 100 similar decisions based on 
these data … 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 
5 10 15
… we will be wrong (make a Type I error) less than 5 
times. 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 
5 10 15
One advantage that statisticians have over juries is that 
we can estimate the probability of Type I error while 
they cannot. 
I can estimate 
the probability 
of being right or 
wrong 
Not sure of the 
probability of 
being right or 
wrong
(Or, at least it is easier for us to do so than for them. 
There is some recent research in rape cases that has 
estimated how frequently juries make Type I errors in 
such cases.)
Even so, we do not get to make the similar decision 100 
times.
We tend to make the decision once. The haunting 
reality is that we never know in this one decision 
whether it is one of the probably occurring Type I 
errors.
In other words, we take a sample of 30 persons and get 
a score of 7. 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
15 16 
5 10 15
In other words, we take a sample of 30 persons and get 
a score of 7. And then another sample and get a score 
of 12, and another with a score of 11, and so on and so 
on until the distribution below emerges. 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
15 16 
5 10 15
But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample 
of 30 for our research purposes, 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
14 
14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 
5 10 15
But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample 
of 30 for our research purposes, 
10 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
14 
14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 
5 10 15
But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample 
of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the 
sample was selected from the far left of the distribution 
below 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
14 
14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 
06 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 
5 10 15
But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample 
of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the 
sample was selected from the far left of the distribution 
below or the far right 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
14 
14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 
5 10 15 
16
But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample 
of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the 
sample was selected from the far left of the distribution 
below or the far right or the middle. 
10 11 
11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
14 
14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 
5 10 15
But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample 
of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the 
sample was selected from the far left of the distribution 
below or the far right or the middle. So, we examine 
the probability that the sample did or did not come 
from the far left or the far right. 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
14 
14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 
5 10 15
But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample 
of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the 
sample was selected from the far left of the distribution 
below or the far right or the middle. So, we examine 
the probability that the sample did or did not come 
from the far left or the far right. 
10 11 
09 10 11 12 
08 09 10 11 12 13 
07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 
14 
14 
06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 
05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 
5 10 15 
Hmm. . . 
What are 
the chances 
the sample 
came from 
the far 
right or left 
of the 
Distri-bution?
So let’s say we want to know if the students who go to 
a college party are more excited to be there than little 
girls at a birthday party.
Here are the sampling distributions of the excitability 
of young girls at a birthday party.
Let’s say we don’t have the same kind of distribution 
for college student excitability at a party. 
?
We want to know if there is a statistical difference 
between the girls at the birthday party and the 
excitability of college students at a Friday night party.
We randomly select a group of college students at a 
party and measure their levels of excitability.
Our random selection is “13”. 
13
Our random selection is “13”. Since this number does 
not lie in the extreme ends we would reject the null 
hypothesis or render a judgment of “not guilty”. 
13
Our random selection is “13”. Since this number does 
not lie in the extreme ends we would reject the null 
hypothesis or render a judgment of “not guilty”. 
College Students and little girls show no difference. 
13
However, what if we randomly selected a college 
student sample with an average excitability value of 
“05”. 
05
However, what if we randomly selected a college 
student sample with an average excitability value of 
“05”. Wow! This is a rare occurrence. 
05
Because the chance of that happening is so rare we 
would reject the null hypothesis. 
05
Because the chance of that happening is so rare we 
would reject the null hypothesis. We would say 
“guilty!” 
05
Because the chance of that happening is so rare we 
would reject the null hypothesis. We would say 
“guilty!” But if in reality there is no difference, 
05
Because the chance of that happening is so rare we 
would reject the null hypothesis. We would say 
“guilty!” But if in reality there is no difference, then we 
have made a type I error. 
05
Because the chance of that happening is so rare we 
would reject the null hypothesis. We would say 
“guilty!” But if in reality there is no difference, then we 
have made a type I error. 
05 
Researchers are 
willing to take that 
chance.
In conclusion, hypothesis testing, is a way of 
determining the probability of our default position 
(not guilty or no difference) being correct or incorrect.
In conclusion, hypothesis testing, is a way of 
determining the probability of our default position 
(not guilty or no difference) being correct or incorrect. 
We determine the likelihood of being right or wrong 
based on the results.
In conclusion, hypothesis testing, is a way of 
determining the probability of our default position 
(not guilty or no difference) being correct or incorrect. 
We determine the likelihood of being right or wrong 
based on the results. Then we decide if we are willing 
to maintain our default position (no difference) or go 
out on a limb and change our default position (yes 
there is a difference).
What follows are exercises to help you 
check your understanding.
Go as far as you feel you need to until you 
have a good feel for what you know.
First Set of Questions
1. Which expression below from the world of judicial 
decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”?
1. Which expression below from the world of judicial 
decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. “Guilty as charged” 
B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” 
C. “Pleading no contest”
1. Which expression below from the world of judicial 
decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. “Guilty as charged” 
B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” 
C. “Pleading no contest”
1. Which expression below from the world of judicial 
decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. “Guilty as charged” 
B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” 
C. “Pleading no contest” 
2. What is another way to say “Null-hypothesis”?
1. Which expression below from the world of judicial 
decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. “Guilty as charged” 
B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” 
C. “Pleading no contest” 
2. What is another way to say “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. Not clear 
B. Not different 
C. Not important
1. Which expression below from the world of judicial 
decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. “Guilty as charged” 
B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” 
C. “Pleading no contest” 
2. What is another way to say “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. Not clear 
B. Not different 
C. Not important
With hypothesis testing we are attempting to set up a 
default position of not guilty. We stay in that position 
unless we have enough evidence to overturn it.
With hypothesis testing we are attempting to set up a 
default position of not guilty. We stay in that position 
unless we have enough evidence to overturn it. 
Let’s say our null-hypothesis is the following:
With hypothesis testing we are attempting to set up a 
default position of not guilty. We stay in that position 
unless we have enough evidence to overturn it. 
Let’s say our null-hypothesis is the following: 
There is no difference in IQ between children who are 
exposed to classical music between the ages of 0 and 3 
and those who were not.
With hypothesis testing we are attempting to set up a 
default position of not guilty. We stay in that position 
unless we have enough evidence to overturn it. 
Let’s say our null-hypothesis is the following: 
There is no difference in IQ between children who are 
exposed to classical music between the ages of 0 and 3 
and those who were not. 
This is our default position. We are not neutral, we are 
claiming at the outset that there is no difference.
But then along comes some evidence that over turns that 
position. So we reject the null hypothesis and claim there 
is a probable difference.
But then along comes some evidence that over turns that 
position. So we reject the null hypothesis and claim there 
is a probable difference. 
Notice how we don’t say “there is a difference”. We say 
there is a probable or statistical difference. This just 
means that with statistics we are never 100% certain. We 
just say that the probability that we are wrong is a certain 
percent. Usually that percent needs to be pretty low.
If we have estimated that there is a 60% chance that we 
are wrong, that is a risk not worth taking. If you were told 
that you had a 60% chance of losing a lot of money and a 
40% chance of making a lot of money, would you take that 
chance? 
Probably not. But if you were told that you had only a 5% 
chance of losing a lot of money and a 95% of earning a lot, 
that might be a chance you would be willing to take. The 
same holds true with hypothesis testing.
If we have estimated that there is a 60% chance that we 
are wrong, that is a risk not worth taking. If you were told 
that you had a 60% chance of losing a lot of money and a 
40% chance of making a lot of money, would you take that 
chance? 
Probably not. But if you were told that you had only a 5% 
chance of losing a lot of money and a 95% of earning a lot, 
that might be a chance you would be willing to take. The 
same holds true with hypothesis testing.
Based on that instruction, consider your answer to these 
questions again and explain the correct answer in your 
own words.
Based on that instruction, consider your answer to these 
questions again and explain the correct answer in your 
own words. 
1. Which expression below from the world of judicial 
decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. “Guilty as charged” 
B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” 
C. “Pleading no contest” 
2. What is another way to say “Null-hypothesis”? 
A. Not clear 
B. Not different 
C. Not important
Second Set of Questions – see if you can answer these 
questions, if not go to the instruction that follows and 
you’ll be given an opportunity to respond to the 
questions armed with the instruction.
3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so.
3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so.
3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so.
3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so.
3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
4. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so.
3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
4. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so.
3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
4. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so.
3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
4. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so.
5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so.
5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so.
5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so.
5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so.
5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
6. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant really 
is “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so.
5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
6. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant really 
is “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so.
5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
6. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant really 
is “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so.
5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant 
actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as 
saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were wrong to do so. 
6. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant really 
is “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: 
A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so. 
B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you 
were right to do so.
Accepting the null-hypothesis is essentially like saying “not 
guilty” or that we accept the default position of innocence 
or no difference. 
Rejecting the null-hypothesis is essentially like saying 
“guilty” or that we reject the default position of innocence 
or there is enough evidence to suggest there is a 
difference.
Here is a visual:
Here is a visual: 
Null-hypothesis 
ACCEPTED!
Here is a visual: 
Null-hypothesis 
ACCEPTED! 
I was found 
NOT 
GUILTY!
Here is a visual: 
Null-hypothesis 
ACCEPTED! 
I was found 
NOT 
GUILTY! 
Na, na, . . . nanana! There is 
NOT enough statistical 
evidence to convict or reject 
the null-hypothesis!
Here is a visual: 
Null-hypothesis 
ACCEPTED! 
I was found 
NOT 
GUILTY! 
Na, na, . . . nanana! There is 
NOT enough statistical 
evidence to convict or reject 
the null-hypothesis! 
Not Guilty = Accept the Null
Here is a visual: 
Null-hypothesis 
REJECTED!
Here is a visual: 
Null-hypothesis 
REJECTED! 
I was 
found 
GUILTY!
Here is a visual: 
Null-hypothesis 
REJECTED! 
I was 
found 
GUILTY! 
Wa, Wa! There IS enough 
statistical evidence to 
convict or reject the null- 
Hypothesis!
Here is a visual: 
Null-hypothesis 
REJECTED! 
I was 
found 
GUILTY! 
Wa, Wa! There IS enough 
statistical evidence to 
convict or reject the null- 
Hypothesis! 
Guilty = Reject the Null
Third Set of Questions - see if you can answer these 
questions, if not go to the instruction that follows and 
you’ll be given an opportunity to respond to the questions 
armed with the instruction.
7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the 
defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have 
rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the 
defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have 
rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
8. When the jury decides “not guilty” (accept the null) and 
the defendant actually was “guilty” (reject the null), what 
type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
10. Question: What is the haunting reality? 
Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have 
committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is 
determining the probability that we . . . 
have committed an error. 
are correct in our hypothesis.
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
10. Question: What is the haunting reality? 
Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have 
committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is 
determining the probability that we . . . 
have committed an error. 
are correct in our hypothesis.
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
10. Question: What is the haunting reality? 
Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have 
committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is 
determining the probability that we . . . 
A. have committed an error. 
B. are correct in our hypothesis.
Let’s consider each type of error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence, 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis, 
4. You accept the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have rejected the null. 
6. This is a type I error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis, 
4. You accept the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have rejected the null. 
6. This is a type I error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You accept the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have rejected the null. 
6. This is a type I error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You accept the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have rejected the null. 
6. This is a type I error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You accept the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have rejected the null. 
6. This is a type I error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You accept the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have rejected the null. 
This is a type I error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You reject the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have accepted the null 
This is a type II error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You reject the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have accepted the null 
This is a type II error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You reject the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have accepted the null 
This is a type II error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You reject the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have accepted the null 
This is a type II error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You reject the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have accepted the null 
This is a type II error
1. State your null-hypothesis; 
There is no significant difference between females and 
males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car 
colors 
2. Collect your evidence 
3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting 
the null-hypothesis 
4. You reject the null 
5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you 
were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between 
men and women sports-car color preference and you 
should have accepted the null 
This is a type II error
You’ll never know if you 
committed a type I or II error. 
You can only estimate the 
probability that you did!
That’s because with 
statistics we deal in 
probability, not 
certainty.
Based on the instruction you just received, respond to 
these questions again. Explain your reasoning for selecting 
the options you did.
7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the 
defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have 
rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the 
defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have 
rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the 
defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have 
rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
8. When the jury decides “not guilty” (accept the null) and 
the defendant actually was “guilty” (reject the null), what 
type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the 
defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have 
rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
8. When the jury decides “not guilty” (accept the null) and 
the defendant actually was “guilty” (reject the null), what 
type of error has been committed? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
10. Question: What is the haunting reality? 
Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have 
committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is 
determining the probability that we . . . 
have committed an error. 
are correct in our hypothesis.
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
10. Question: What is the haunting reality? 
Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have 
committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is 
determining the probability that we . . . 
have committed an error. 
are correct in our hypothesis.
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
10. Question: What is the haunting reality? 
Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have 
committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is 
determining the probability that we . . . 
A. have committed an error. 
B. are correct in our hypothesis. 
Answers: 7-A, 8-B, 9-B, 10-A
9. Which type of error is preferable? 
A. Type I error 
B. Type II error 
10. Question: What is the haunting reality? 
Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have 
committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is 
determining the probability that we . . . 
A. have committed an error. 
B. are correct in our hypothesis. 
Answers: 7-A, 8-B, 9-B, 10-A
Fourth Set of Questions - see if you can answer these 
questions, if not go to the instruction that follows and 
you’ll be given an opportunity to respond to the questions 
armed with the instruction.
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong 
12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? 
Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 
times from a population, we would be willing to 
_____________________ .05 or 5% of the time 
a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . 
b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . .
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong 
12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? 
Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 
times from a population, we would be willing to 
_____________________ .05 or 5% of the time 
a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . 
b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . .
In statistics we generally ask ourselves, “What is the 
probability that we have made a type I error?”
In statistics we generally ask ourselves, “What is the 
probability that we have made a Type I Error?” 
Type I errors are considered a bigger issue because if we 
are wrong, than we might waste a lot of money or impact 
people negatively (e.g., spend millions of dollars on a new 
drug that doesn’t work).
In statistics we generally ask ourselves, “What is the 
probability that we have made a Type I Error?” 
Type I errors are considered a bigger issue because if we 
are wrong, than we might waste a lot of money or impact 
people negatively (e.g., spend millions of dollars on a new 
drug that doesn’t work). 
Type II errors are considered less of an issue because if we 
are wrong, than we may stop or continue researching.
We have to have determine a cut-off point as to when we 
will reject the null-hypothesis. No matter what cut-off 
point we could have chosen, the decision would always 
have been somewhat arbitrary.
We have to have determine a cut-off point as to when we 
will reject the null-hypothesis. No matter what cut-off 
point we could have chosen, the decision would always 
have been somewhat arbitrary. 
Would we be satisfied with a 75% chance of committing a 
type I error? Probably not. That means out of 100 
experiments we would live with being wrong about our 
conclusions 75 times.
Would we be satisfied with a .01% chance of committing a 
type I error? Probably not. That means out of 10,000 
experiments we would live with being wrong about our 
conclusions only once. If that were the case, then almost 
no null-hypothesis could ever be rejected.
Would we be satisfied with a .01% chance of committing a 
type I error? Probably not. That means out of 10,000 
experiments we would live with being wrong about our 
conclusions only once. If that were the case, then almost 
no null-hypothesis could ever be rejected. 
In the discipline of statistics .05 or 5% of a chance of 
committing a type I error has been deemed an acceptable 
arbitrary cut-off point. This means that out of 100 
experiments we will live with being wrong five times.
Based on the instruction you just received, respond to 
these questions again. Explain your reasoning for selecting 
the options you did.
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong 
12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? 
Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 
times from a population, we would be willing to 
_____________________ .05 or 5% of the time 
a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . 
b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . .
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong 
12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? 
Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 
times from a population, we would be willing to 
_____________________ .05 or 5% of the time 
a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . 
b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . . Answers: 11-B, 12-A
11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is 
required before we will reject the null hypothesis? 
Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a 
certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). 
a. right 
b. wrong 
12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? 
Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 
times from a population, we would be willing to 
_____________________ .05 or 5% of the time 
a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . 
b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . . Answers: 11-B, 12-A

More Related Content

What's hot

Hypothesis testing
Hypothesis testingHypothesis testing
Hypothesis testingArnab Sadhu
 
Testing Of Hypothesis
Testing Of HypothesisTesting Of Hypothesis
Testing Of HypothesisSWATI SINGH
 
Statistical inference concept, procedure of hypothesis testing
Statistical inference   concept, procedure of hypothesis testingStatistical inference   concept, procedure of hypothesis testing
Statistical inference concept, procedure of hypothesis testingAmitaChaudhary19
 
Test of hypothesis
Test of hypothesisTest of hypothesis
Test of hypothesisvikramlawand
 
Testing of hypotheses
Testing of hypothesesTesting of hypotheses
Testing of hypothesesRajThakuri
 
One Sample T Test
One Sample T TestOne Sample T Test
One Sample T Testshoffma5
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis Testingrishi.indian
 
Basis of statistical inference
Basis of statistical inferenceBasis of statistical inference
Basis of statistical inferencezahidacademy
 
Review & Hypothesis Testing
Review & Hypothesis TestingReview & Hypothesis Testing
Review & Hypothesis TestingSr Edith Bogue
 
Lecture2 hypothesis testing
Lecture2 hypothesis testingLecture2 hypothesis testing
Lecture2 hypothesis testingo_devinyak
 
Hypothesis and Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis and Hypothesis TestingHypothesis and Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis and Hypothesis TestingNaibin
 
Hypothesis testing an introduction
Hypothesis testing an introductionHypothesis testing an introduction
Hypothesis testing an introductionGeetika Gulyani
 
PROCEDURE FOR TESTING HYPOTHESIS
PROCEDURE FOR   TESTING HYPOTHESIS PROCEDURE FOR   TESTING HYPOTHESIS
PROCEDURE FOR TESTING HYPOTHESIS Sundar B N
 

What's hot (20)

Hypothesis testing
Hypothesis testingHypothesis testing
Hypothesis testing
 
Hypothesis testing
Hypothesis testingHypothesis testing
Hypothesis testing
 
HYPOTHESIS TESTING.ppt
HYPOTHESIS TESTING.pptHYPOTHESIS TESTING.ppt
HYPOTHESIS TESTING.ppt
 
Testing Of Hypothesis
Testing Of HypothesisTesting Of Hypothesis
Testing Of Hypothesis
 
Statistical inference concept, procedure of hypothesis testing
Statistical inference   concept, procedure of hypothesis testingStatistical inference   concept, procedure of hypothesis testing
Statistical inference concept, procedure of hypothesis testing
 
Test of hypothesis
Test of hypothesisTest of hypothesis
Test of hypothesis
 
Testing of hypotheses
Testing of hypothesesTesting of hypotheses
Testing of hypotheses
 
One Sample T Test
One Sample T TestOne Sample T Test
One Sample T Test
 
Hypothesis
HypothesisHypothesis
Hypothesis
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis Testing
 
Basis of statistical inference
Basis of statistical inferenceBasis of statistical inference
Basis of statistical inference
 
Review & Hypothesis Testing
Review & Hypothesis TestingReview & Hypothesis Testing
Review & Hypothesis Testing
 
Basics of Hypothesis Testing
Basics of Hypothesis Testing  Basics of Hypothesis Testing
Basics of Hypothesis Testing
 
Hypothesis testing
Hypothesis testingHypothesis testing
Hypothesis testing
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis Testing
 
Two sample t-test
Two sample t-testTwo sample t-test
Two sample t-test
 
Lecture2 hypothesis testing
Lecture2 hypothesis testingLecture2 hypothesis testing
Lecture2 hypothesis testing
 
Hypothesis and Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis and Hypothesis TestingHypothesis and Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis and Hypothesis Testing
 
Hypothesis testing an introduction
Hypothesis testing an introductionHypothesis testing an introduction
Hypothesis testing an introduction
 
PROCEDURE FOR TESTING HYPOTHESIS
PROCEDURE FOR   TESTING HYPOTHESIS PROCEDURE FOR   TESTING HYPOTHESIS
PROCEDURE FOR TESTING HYPOTHESIS
 

Viewers also liked

Chapter 8 – Hypothesis Testing
Chapter 8 – Hypothesis TestingChapter 8 – Hypothesis Testing
Chapter 8 – Hypothesis TestingRose Jenkins
 
Hypothesis testing ppt final
Hypothesis testing ppt finalHypothesis testing ppt final
Hypothesis testing ppt finalpiyushdhaker
 
Hypothesis testing; z test, t-test. f-test
Hypothesis testing; z test, t-test. f-testHypothesis testing; z test, t-test. f-test
Hypothesis testing; z test, t-test. f-testShakehand with Life
 
Session 9 intro_of_topics_in_hypothesis_testing
Session 9 intro_of_topics_in_hypothesis_testingSession 9 intro_of_topics_in_hypothesis_testing
Session 9 intro_of_topics_in_hypothesis_testingGlory Codilla
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingJeremy Lane
 
hypothesis testing
hypothesis testinghypothesis testing
hypothesis testingmsrpt
 
Statistical inference: Hypothesis Testing and t-tests
Statistical inference: Hypothesis Testing and t-testsStatistical inference: Hypothesis Testing and t-tests
Statistical inference: Hypothesis Testing and t-testsEugene Yan Ziyou
 
Sample to sample or sample to population
Sample to sample or sample to populationSample to sample or sample to population
Sample to sample or sample to populationKen Plummer
 
Missing data eliminate cases
Missing data   eliminate casesMissing data   eliminate cases
Missing data eliminate casesKen Plummer
 
Is there a covariate?
Is there a covariate?Is there a covariate?
Is there a covariate?Ken Plummer
 
What single samples t test (2)?
What single samples t test (2)?What single samples t test (2)?
What single samples t test (2)?Ken Plummer
 
Null hypothesis for phi-coefficient
Null hypothesis for phi-coefficientNull hypothesis for phi-coefficient
Null hypothesis for phi-coefficientKen Plummer
 
Is the data nominal tallied, or ordinal (ranked)?
Is the data nominal tallied, or ordinal (ranked)?Is the data nominal tallied, or ordinal (ranked)?
Is the data nominal tallied, or ordinal (ranked)?Ken Plummer
 
Identifying predictable patterns
Identifying predictable patternsIdentifying predictable patterns
Identifying predictable patternsKen Plummer
 
Null hypothesis for a single-sample t-test
Null hypothesis for a single-sample t-testNull hypothesis for a single-sample t-test
Null hypothesis for a single-sample t-testKen Plummer
 

Viewers also liked (20)

Chapter 8 – Hypothesis Testing
Chapter 8 – Hypothesis TestingChapter 8 – Hypothesis Testing
Chapter 8 – Hypothesis Testing
 
Hypothesis testing ppt final
Hypothesis testing ppt finalHypothesis testing ppt final
Hypothesis testing ppt final
 
Hypothesis
HypothesisHypothesis
Hypothesis
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis Testing
 
Hypothesis testing; z test, t-test. f-test
Hypothesis testing; z test, t-test. f-testHypothesis testing; z test, t-test. f-test
Hypothesis testing; z test, t-test. f-test
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis Testing
 
Session 9 intro_of_topics_in_hypothesis_testing
Session 9 intro_of_topics_in_hypothesis_testingSession 9 intro_of_topics_in_hypothesis_testing
Session 9 intro_of_topics_in_hypothesis_testing
 
Hypothesis testing
Hypothesis testingHypothesis testing
Hypothesis testing
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis Testing
 
hypothesis testing
hypothesis testinghypothesis testing
hypothesis testing
 
Hypothesis testing
Hypothesis testingHypothesis testing
Hypothesis testing
 
Statistical inference: Hypothesis Testing and t-tests
Statistical inference: Hypothesis Testing and t-testsStatistical inference: Hypothesis Testing and t-tests
Statistical inference: Hypothesis Testing and t-tests
 
Sample to sample or sample to population
Sample to sample or sample to populationSample to sample or sample to population
Sample to sample or sample to population
 
Missing data eliminate cases
Missing data   eliminate casesMissing data   eliminate cases
Missing data eliminate cases
 
Is there a covariate?
Is there a covariate?Is there a covariate?
Is there a covariate?
 
What single samples t test (2)?
What single samples t test (2)?What single samples t test (2)?
What single samples t test (2)?
 
Null hypothesis for phi-coefficient
Null hypothesis for phi-coefficientNull hypothesis for phi-coefficient
Null hypothesis for phi-coefficient
 
Is the data nominal tallied, or ordinal (ranked)?
Is the data nominal tallied, or ordinal (ranked)?Is the data nominal tallied, or ordinal (ranked)?
Is the data nominal tallied, or ordinal (ranked)?
 
Identifying predictable patterns
Identifying predictable patternsIdentifying predictable patterns
Identifying predictable patterns
 
Null hypothesis for a single-sample t-test
Null hypothesis for a single-sample t-testNull hypothesis for a single-sample t-test
Null hypothesis for a single-sample t-test
 

Similar to Hypothesis Testing

Introduction to Philosophy - Truth and knowledge
Introduction to Philosophy - Truth and knowledge Introduction to Philosophy - Truth and knowledge
Introduction to Philosophy - Truth and knowledge AudreyFlores4
 
Communicating With Jurors About Money Seminar Power Point
Communicating With Jurors About Money  Seminar Power PointCommunicating With Jurors About Money  Seminar Power Point
Communicating With Jurors About Money Seminar Power Pointmccormick
 
BASIC LAW CONCEPTS: OFFENCES & DEFENSES
BASIC LAW CONCEPTS: OFFENCES & DEFENSESBASIC LAW CONCEPTS: OFFENCES & DEFENSES
BASIC LAW CONCEPTS: OFFENCES & DEFENSESVirginia Westerberg
 
Evaluate Your Argument on the IssueIn this chapter you will lear.docx
Evaluate Your Argument on the IssueIn this chapter you will lear.docxEvaluate Your Argument on the IssueIn this chapter you will lear.docx
Evaluate Your Argument on the IssueIn this chapter you will lear.docxgitagrimston
 
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docxTexas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Research methodology hypothesis construction
Research methodology   hypothesis constructionResearch methodology   hypothesis construction
Research methodology hypothesis constructionSami Nighaoui
 
Extra Credit ( Evidence)
Extra Credit ( Evidence)Extra Credit ( Evidence)
Extra Credit ( Evidence)Roxana Chavez
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingSampath
 
Logical Fallacies Slide Show
Logical Fallacies Slide Show Logical Fallacies Slide Show
Logical Fallacies Slide Show Crowder College
 
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docxTexas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docxmehek4
 
I’m Not Guilty, Because I Had No ChoiceWe were talking last week.docx
I’m Not Guilty, Because I Had No ChoiceWe were talking last week.docxI’m Not Guilty, Because I Had No ChoiceWe were talking last week.docx
I’m Not Guilty, Because I Had No ChoiceWe were talking last week.docxssuser47f0be
 
What is the false confession and how to copy
What is the false confession and how to   copyWhat is the false confession and how to   copy
What is the false confession and how to copyMohammad Rahat
 
132 People In Jail Essay
132 People In Jail Essay132 People In Jail Essay
132 People In Jail EssayKristin Oliver
 
National CUPA-HR Presentation
National CUPA-HR PresentationNational CUPA-HR Presentation
National CUPA-HR PresentationGlenn Powell
 
What is the role of a jury in a criminal case Is it important or.pdf
 What is the role of a jury in a criminal case Is it important or.pdf What is the role of a jury in a criminal case Is it important or.pdf
What is the role of a jury in a criminal case Is it important or.pdfalrahmancollection
 

Similar to Hypothesis Testing (20)

Philosophy homework help
Philosophy homework helpPhilosophy homework help
Philosophy homework help
 
Introduction to Philosophy - Truth and knowledge
Introduction to Philosophy - Truth and knowledge Introduction to Philosophy - Truth and knowledge
Introduction to Philosophy - Truth and knowledge
 
Communicating With Jurors About Money Seminar Power Point
Communicating With Jurors About Money  Seminar Power PointCommunicating With Jurors About Money  Seminar Power Point
Communicating With Jurors About Money Seminar Power Point
 
BASIC LAW CONCEPTS: OFFENCES & DEFENSES
BASIC LAW CONCEPTS: OFFENCES & DEFENSESBASIC LAW CONCEPTS: OFFENCES & DEFENSES
BASIC LAW CONCEPTS: OFFENCES & DEFENSES
 
Evaluate Your Argument on the IssueIn this chapter you will lear.docx
Evaluate Your Argument on the IssueIn this chapter you will lear.docxEvaluate Your Argument on the IssueIn this chapter you will lear.docx
Evaluate Your Argument on the IssueIn this chapter you will lear.docx
 
Fallacies
FallaciesFallacies
Fallacies
 
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docxTexas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
 
Research methodology hypothesis construction
Research methodology   hypothesis constructionResearch methodology   hypothesis construction
Research methodology hypothesis construction
 
Extra Credit ( Evidence)
Extra Credit ( Evidence)Extra Credit ( Evidence)
Extra Credit ( Evidence)
 
Hypothesis Testing
Hypothesis TestingHypothesis Testing
Hypothesis Testing
 
Logical Fallacies Slide Show
Logical Fallacies Slide Show Logical Fallacies Slide Show
Logical Fallacies Slide Show
 
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docxTexas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
Texas Government OnlineDeath Penalty Paper ProjectCindy Case.docx
 
Mr 2013 14
Mr 2013 14Mr 2013 14
Mr 2013 14
 
Burden of proof
Burden of proofBurden of proof
Burden of proof
 
I’m Not Guilty, Because I Had No ChoiceWe were talking last week.docx
I’m Not Guilty, Because I Had No ChoiceWe were talking last week.docxI’m Not Guilty, Because I Had No ChoiceWe were talking last week.docx
I’m Not Guilty, Because I Had No ChoiceWe were talking last week.docx
 
What is the false confession and how to copy
What is the false confession and how to   copyWhat is the false confession and how to   copy
What is the false confession and how to copy
 
132 People In Jail Essay
132 People In Jail Essay132 People In Jail Essay
132 People In Jail Essay
 
National CUPA-HR Presentation
National CUPA-HR PresentationNational CUPA-HR Presentation
National CUPA-HR Presentation
 
What is the role of a jury in a criminal case Is it important or.pdf
 What is the role of a jury in a criminal case Is it important or.pdf What is the role of a jury in a criminal case Is it important or.pdf
What is the role of a jury in a criminal case Is it important or.pdf
 
Mens Rea
Mens ReaMens Rea
Mens Rea
 

More from Ken Plummer

Diff rel gof-fit - jejit - practice (5)
Diff rel gof-fit - jejit - practice (5)Diff rel gof-fit - jejit - practice (5)
Diff rel gof-fit - jejit - practice (5)Ken Plummer
 
Learn About Range - Copyright updated
Learn About Range - Copyright updatedLearn About Range - Copyright updated
Learn About Range - Copyright updatedKen Plummer
 
Inferential vs descriptive tutorial of when to use - Copyright Updated
Inferential vs descriptive tutorial of when to use - Copyright UpdatedInferential vs descriptive tutorial of when to use - Copyright Updated
Inferential vs descriptive tutorial of when to use - Copyright UpdatedKen Plummer
 
Diff rel ind-fit practice - Copyright Updated
Diff rel ind-fit practice - Copyright UpdatedDiff rel ind-fit practice - Copyright Updated
Diff rel ind-fit practice - Copyright UpdatedKen Plummer
 
Normal or skewed distributions (inferential) - Copyright updated
Normal or skewed distributions (inferential) - Copyright updatedNormal or skewed distributions (inferential) - Copyright updated
Normal or skewed distributions (inferential) - Copyright updatedKen Plummer
 
Normal or skewed distributions (descriptive both2) - Copyright updated
Normal or skewed distributions (descriptive both2) - Copyright updatedNormal or skewed distributions (descriptive both2) - Copyright updated
Normal or skewed distributions (descriptive both2) - Copyright updatedKen Plummer
 
Nature of the data practice - Copyright updated
Nature of the data practice - Copyright updatedNature of the data practice - Copyright updated
Nature of the data practice - Copyright updatedKen Plummer
 
Nature of the data (spread) - Copyright updated
Nature of the data (spread) - Copyright updatedNature of the data (spread) - Copyright updated
Nature of the data (spread) - Copyright updatedKen Plummer
 
Mode practice 1 - Copyright updated
Mode practice 1 - Copyright updatedMode practice 1 - Copyright updated
Mode practice 1 - Copyright updatedKen Plummer
 
Nature of the data (descriptive) - Copyright updated
Nature of the data (descriptive) - Copyright updatedNature of the data (descriptive) - Copyright updated
Nature of the data (descriptive) - Copyright updatedKen Plummer
 
Dichotomous or scaled
Dichotomous or scaledDichotomous or scaled
Dichotomous or scaledKen Plummer
 
Skewed less than 30 (ties)
Skewed less than 30 (ties)Skewed less than 30 (ties)
Skewed less than 30 (ties)Ken Plummer
 
Skewed sample size less than 30
Skewed sample size less than 30Skewed sample size less than 30
Skewed sample size less than 30Ken Plummer
 
Ordinal and nominal
Ordinal and nominalOrdinal and nominal
Ordinal and nominalKen Plummer
 
Relationship covariates
Relationship   covariatesRelationship   covariates
Relationship covariatesKen Plummer
 
Relationship nature of data
Relationship nature of dataRelationship nature of data
Relationship nature of dataKen Plummer
 
Number of variables (predictive)
Number of variables (predictive)Number of variables (predictive)
Number of variables (predictive)Ken Plummer
 
Levels of the iv
Levels of the ivLevels of the iv
Levels of the ivKen Plummer
 
Independent variables (2)
Independent variables (2)Independent variables (2)
Independent variables (2)Ken Plummer
 

More from Ken Plummer (20)

Diff rel gof-fit - jejit - practice (5)
Diff rel gof-fit - jejit - practice (5)Diff rel gof-fit - jejit - practice (5)
Diff rel gof-fit - jejit - practice (5)
 
Learn About Range - Copyright updated
Learn About Range - Copyright updatedLearn About Range - Copyright updated
Learn About Range - Copyright updated
 
Inferential vs descriptive tutorial of when to use - Copyright Updated
Inferential vs descriptive tutorial of when to use - Copyright UpdatedInferential vs descriptive tutorial of when to use - Copyright Updated
Inferential vs descriptive tutorial of when to use - Copyright Updated
 
Diff rel ind-fit practice - Copyright Updated
Diff rel ind-fit practice - Copyright UpdatedDiff rel ind-fit practice - Copyright Updated
Diff rel ind-fit practice - Copyright Updated
 
Normal or skewed distributions (inferential) - Copyright updated
Normal or skewed distributions (inferential) - Copyright updatedNormal or skewed distributions (inferential) - Copyright updated
Normal or skewed distributions (inferential) - Copyright updated
 
Normal or skewed distributions (descriptive both2) - Copyright updated
Normal or skewed distributions (descriptive both2) - Copyright updatedNormal or skewed distributions (descriptive both2) - Copyright updated
Normal or skewed distributions (descriptive both2) - Copyright updated
 
Nature of the data practice - Copyright updated
Nature of the data practice - Copyright updatedNature of the data practice - Copyright updated
Nature of the data practice - Copyright updated
 
Nature of the data (spread) - Copyright updated
Nature of the data (spread) - Copyright updatedNature of the data (spread) - Copyright updated
Nature of the data (spread) - Copyright updated
 
Mode practice 1 - Copyright updated
Mode practice 1 - Copyright updatedMode practice 1 - Copyright updated
Mode practice 1 - Copyright updated
 
Nature of the data (descriptive) - Copyright updated
Nature of the data (descriptive) - Copyright updatedNature of the data (descriptive) - Copyright updated
Nature of the data (descriptive) - Copyright updated
 
Dichotomous or scaled
Dichotomous or scaledDichotomous or scaled
Dichotomous or scaled
 
Skewed less than 30 (ties)
Skewed less than 30 (ties)Skewed less than 30 (ties)
Skewed less than 30 (ties)
 
Skewed sample size less than 30
Skewed sample size less than 30Skewed sample size less than 30
Skewed sample size less than 30
 
Ordinal (ties)
Ordinal (ties)Ordinal (ties)
Ordinal (ties)
 
Ordinal and nominal
Ordinal and nominalOrdinal and nominal
Ordinal and nominal
 
Relationship covariates
Relationship   covariatesRelationship   covariates
Relationship covariates
 
Relationship nature of data
Relationship nature of dataRelationship nature of data
Relationship nature of data
 
Number of variables (predictive)
Number of variables (predictive)Number of variables (predictive)
Number of variables (predictive)
 
Levels of the iv
Levels of the ivLevels of the iv
Levels of the iv
 
Independent variables (2)
Independent variables (2)Independent variables (2)
Independent variables (2)
 

Recently uploaded

ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfSpandanaRallapalli
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomnelietumpap1
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parentsnavabharathschool99
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Celine George
 
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYKayeClaireEstoconing
 
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipinoFILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipinojohnmickonozaleda
 
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdfVirtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdfErwinPantujan2
 
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)lakshayb543
 
Concurrency Control in Database Management system
Concurrency Control in Database Management systemConcurrency Control in Database Management system
Concurrency Control in Database Management systemChristalin Nelson
 
Transaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemTransaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemChristalin Nelson
 
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Mark Reed
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONHumphrey A Beña
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxKarra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxAshokKarra1
 
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxCulture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxPoojaSen20
 
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...JhezDiaz1
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptxSherlyMaeNeri
 

Recently uploaded (20)

ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
 
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
 
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipinoFILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
FILIPINO PSYCHology sikolohiyang pilipino
 
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdfVirtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
 
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
 
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxFINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
Concurrency Control in Database Management system
Concurrency Control in Database Management systemConcurrency Control in Database Management system
Concurrency Control in Database Management system
 
Transaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management SystemTransaction Management in Database Management System
Transaction Management in Database Management System
 
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptxKarra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
Karra SKD Conference Presentation Revised.pptx
 
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptxCulture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
Culture Uniformity or Diversity IN SOCIOLOGY.pptx
 
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 3 STEPS Using Odoo 17
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
 

Hypothesis Testing

  • 2. The logic of statistical hypothesis testing follows the logic of judicial decision making.
  • 3. A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty or not guilty.
  • 4. A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous decision, guilty or not guilty.
  • 5. A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous decision, guilty or not guilty. There is no in-between or partial decision.
  • 6. A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous decision, guilty or not guilty. There is no in-between or partial decision. The jury does not begin its decision-making process in a neutral position.
  • 7. A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous decision, guilty or not guilty. There is no in-between or partial decision. The jury does not begin its decision-making process in a neutral position. The default position is “not guilty.”
  • 8. A jury is asked to decide whether a defendant is guilty or not guilty. It is a dicho-tomous decision, guilty or not guilty. There is no in-between or partial decision. The jury does not begin its decision-making process in a neutral position. The default position is “not guilty.” The prosecution must mount enough evidence to convince the jury to move from its default position of not guilty to a verdict of guilty.
  • 9. The jury will make a decision which may or may not coincide with reality.
  • 10. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, not guilty, It is true because the not guilty (negative) decision aligns with the not guilty (negative) reality.
  • 11. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, not guilty, they have made a correct decision called a “true negative decision.” It is true because the not guilty (negative) decision aligns with the not guilty (negative) reality.
  • 12. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, not guilty, they have made a correct decision called a “true negative decision.” It is true because the not guilty (negative) decision aligns with the not guilty (negative) reality. not guilty and I really wasn’t guilty! true negative
  • 13. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, guilty, It is true because the guilty (positive) decision aligns with the guilty (positive) reality.
  • 14. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, guilty, they have made a correct decision called a “true positive” decision. It is true because the guilty (positive) decision aligns with the guilty (positive) reality.
  • 15. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, guilty, they have made a correct decision called a “true positive” decision. It is true because the guilty (positive) decision aligns with the guilty (positive) reality. guilty and I really WAS guilty! true positive
  • 16. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, guilty,
  • 17. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, guilty, they have made an incorrect decision called a “false negative error” which is also called a Type II or beta error.
  • 18. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, guilty, they have made an incorrect decision called a “false negative error” which is also called a Type II or beta error. It is false because the “not guilty” (negative) decision does not align with the guilty (positive) reality. not guilty and I really WAS guilty! false negative
  • 19. When a jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, not guilty,
  • 20. When a jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, not guilty, they have made an incorrect decision called a “false positive error”
  • 21. When a jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, not guilty, they have made an incorrect decision called a “false positive error” which is also called a Type I or alpha error.
  • 22. When a jury decides “guilty” and the defendant is, in reality, not guilty, they have made an incorrect decision called a “false positive error” which is also called a Type I or alpha error. It is false because the “guilty” (positive) decision is not aligned with the not guilty (negative) reality. guilty but I really WASN’T guilty! false positive
  • 23. Although we prefer correct decisions, if we cannot be correct, we prefer the false negative error over the false positive error. In other words you’d rather render a “NOT GUILTY” verdict when there is GUILT. Than a “GUILTY” verdict where there is NO GUILT.
  • 24. Although we prefer correct decisions, if we cannot be correct, we prefer the false negative error over the false positive error. In other words you’d rather render a “NOT GUILTY” verdict when there is GUILT. not guilty and I really WAS guilty! Than a “GUILTY” verdict where there is NO GUILT.
  • 25. Although we prefer correct decisions, if we cannot be correct, we prefer the false negative error over the false positive error. In other words you’d rather render a “NOT GUILTY” verdict when there is GUILT. not guilty and I really WAS guilty! Than a “GUILTY” verdict where there is NO GUILT. guilty but I really WASN’T guilty!
  • 26. In judicial decisions we would rather let a guilty defendant go free . . . than convict and imprison an innocent defendant. Our default position of “not guilty” supports this preference and protects against the least favorable condition.
  • 27. In judicial decisions we would rather let a guilty defendant go free . . . than convict and imprison an innocent defendant. Our default position of “not guilty” supports this preference and protects against the least favorable condition.
  • 28. In judicial decisions we would rather let a guilty defendant go free . . . than convict and imprison an innocent defendant. Our default position of “not guilty” supports this preference and protects against the least favorable condition.
  • 29. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow:
  • 30. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow: What type of decision is made when a guilty (+) verdict is rendered and the person is guilty (+)?
  • 31. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow: What type of decision is made when a guilty (+) verdict is rendered and the person is guilty (+)?
  • 32. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow: What type of decision is made when a not guilty (-) verdict is rendered and the person is not guilty (-)?
  • 33. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow: What type of decision is made when a not guilty (-) verdict is rendered and the person is not guilty (-)?
  • 34. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow: What type of decision is made when a guilty (+) verdict is rendered and the person is not guilty (-)?
  • 35. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow: What type of decision is made when a guilty (+) verdict is rendered and the person is not guilty (-)?
  • 36. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow: What type of decision is made when a not guilty (-) verdict is rendered and the person is guilty (+) ?
  • 37. Review the following slide and answer the questions that follow: What type of decision is made when a not guilty (-) verdict is rendered and the person is guilty (+) ?
  • 38. Each conviction protects against Type I error at a different stringency according to the gravity of the punishment to be imposed.
  • 39. The haunting reality is that we really never know the reality of the guilt or innocence of defendants. We make our best decisions knowing that there is a probability that we have made an error.
  • 40. The haunting reality is that we really never know the reality of the guilt or innocence of defendants. We make our best decisions knowing that there is a probability that we have made an error.
  • 41. Statistical hypothesis testing and decision-making are directly analogous to judicial decision making. Judicial Decisions Statistical Decisions
  • 42. Let’s consider an example: A statistician is asked to decide whether a difference exists between two groups of people in terms of some attribute (e.g., excitability). It is a dichotomous decision (meaning only two options), different or not different. There is no in-between or partial decision.
  • 43. Let’s consider an example: A statistician is asked to decide whether a difference exists between two groups of people in terms of some attribute (e.g., excitability). It is a dichotomous decision (meaning only two options), different or not different. There is no in-between or partial decision.
  • 44. Let’s consider an example: A statistician is asked to decide whether a difference exists between two groups of people in terms of some attribute (e.g., excitability). It is a dichotomous decision (meaning only two options), different or not different. There is no in-between or partial decision. x
  • 45. The statistician does not begin her decision-making in a neutral position. The default position is “not different.” This is also called the “null hypothesis.”
  • 46. The statistician does not begin her decision-making in a neutral position. The default position is “not different.” This is also called the “null hypothesis.”
  • 47. The statistician does not begin her decision-making in a neutral position. The default position is “not different.” This is also called the “null hypothesis.”
  • 48. The research findings must present sufficient evidence to convince the statistician to move from her default position of no difference to a conclusion that the groups are different in terms of the attribute.
  • 49. The statistician will make a decision which may or may not coincide with reality. The apparent differences may be due to chance or may be real.
  • 50. The statistician will make a decision which may or may not coincide with reality. The apparent differences may be due to chance or may be real. OR Something that is really happening
  • 51. When the statistician decides “not different” (fails to reject the null hypothesis, maintains the default position) and the groups are, in reality, not different, she has made a correct decision called a “true negative decision.” true negative
  • 52. It is true because the “no difference” (negative) decision aligns with “no difference” reality. not guilty and I really WASN’T guilty! true negative
  • 53. When the statistician decides that there is a difference (rejects the null hypothesis, moves off of the default position) and the groups are, in reality, different, she has made a correct decision called a true positive decision. true positive
  • 54. It is true because the “different” (positive) decision aligns with the “different” (positive) reality. guilty and I really WAS guilty! true positive
  • 55. When the statistician decides “not different” (fails to reject the null hypothesis, maintains the default position) and the group are, in reality different, she has made a false negative error. false negative
  • 56. It is false because the decision of no difference (negative) does not align with difference (positive) reality. not guilty false negative Ha ha! and I really WAS guilty!
  • 57. Although we prefer correct decisions, if we cannot be correct, we then prefer false negative error over the alternative error.
  • 58. When a statistician decides that there is a difference (positive) between the groups and rejects the null hypothesis of no difference and, in reality, there is no difference, she has made a false positive error (also called Type I error or alpha error.) false positive
  • 59. It is false because the “difference” (positive) decision does not align with the “no difference” (negative) reality. guilty but I really WASN’T guilty! false positive
  • 60. Our hypothesis testing conventions protect against false positive, Type I error by holding a default position of the null hypothesis. α Beware of Type I Error
  • 61. We set a standard of evidence that is required before rejecting the default null hypothesis.
  • 62. The standard of evidence is based on the probability density of the sampling distribution.
  • 63. Using probability density we can estimate the probability of Type I error. If the mean of the sample is here, then we have a .0001 or .01% chance that we made a Type I error.
  • 64. Or in other words, we have a .01% chance of rejecting the null hypothesis that the group scores come from two different populations (claiming guilty) and being wrong when both groups were really part of the same population (not guilty)
  • 65. When the probability of Type I error is at a low enough level, we reject the default, null hypothesis. Like in our previous example.
  • 66. The conventional level of tolerable Type I error is .05. 95% .05 or 5% chance that we selected a sample from this population and claimed it was a sample from another population = false positive
  • 67. This means that out of 100 similar decisions based on these data … 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 5 10 15
  • 68. … we will be wrong (make a Type I error) less than 5 times. 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 5 10 15
  • 69. One advantage that statisticians have over juries is that we can estimate the probability of Type I error while they cannot. I can estimate the probability of being right or wrong Not sure of the probability of being right or wrong
  • 70. (Or, at least it is easier for us to do so than for them. There is some recent research in rape cases that has estimated how frequently juries make Type I errors in such cases.)
  • 71. Even so, we do not get to make the similar decision 100 times.
  • 72. We tend to make the decision once. The haunting reality is that we never know in this one decision whether it is one of the probably occurring Type I errors.
  • 73. In other words, we take a sample of 30 persons and get a score of 7. 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 5 10 15
  • 74. In other words, we take a sample of 30 persons and get a score of 7. And then another sample and get a score of 12, and another with a score of 11, and so on and so on until the distribution below emerges. 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 5 10 15
  • 75. But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample of 30 for our research purposes, 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 14 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 5 10 15
  • 76. But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample of 30 for our research purposes, 10 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 14 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 5 10 15
  • 77. But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the sample was selected from the far left of the distribution below 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 14 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 06 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 5 10 15
  • 78. But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the sample was selected from the far left of the distribution below or the far right 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 14 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 5 10 15 16
  • 79. But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the sample was selected from the far left of the distribution below or the far right or the middle. 10 11 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 14 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 5 10 15
  • 80. But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the sample was selected from the far left of the distribution below or the far right or the middle. So, we examine the probability that the sample did or did not come from the far left or the far right. 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 14 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 5 10 15
  • 81. But since, in real life, we usually only take one sample of 30 for our research purposes, we don’t know if the sample was selected from the far left of the distribution below or the far right or the middle. So, we examine the probability that the sample did or did not come from the far left or the far right. 10 11 09 10 11 12 08 09 10 11 12 13 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 14 14 14 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 05 06 07 08 09 10 11 12 13 15 16 5 10 15 Hmm. . . What are the chances the sample came from the far right or left of the Distri-bution?
  • 82. So let’s say we want to know if the students who go to a college party are more excited to be there than little girls at a birthday party.
  • 83. Here are the sampling distributions of the excitability of young girls at a birthday party.
  • 84. Let’s say we don’t have the same kind of distribution for college student excitability at a party. ?
  • 85. We want to know if there is a statistical difference between the girls at the birthday party and the excitability of college students at a Friday night party.
  • 86. We randomly select a group of college students at a party and measure their levels of excitability.
  • 87. Our random selection is “13”. 13
  • 88. Our random selection is “13”. Since this number does not lie in the extreme ends we would reject the null hypothesis or render a judgment of “not guilty”. 13
  • 89. Our random selection is “13”. Since this number does not lie in the extreme ends we would reject the null hypothesis or render a judgment of “not guilty”. College Students and little girls show no difference. 13
  • 90. However, what if we randomly selected a college student sample with an average excitability value of “05”. 05
  • 91. However, what if we randomly selected a college student sample with an average excitability value of “05”. Wow! This is a rare occurrence. 05
  • 92. Because the chance of that happening is so rare we would reject the null hypothesis. 05
  • 93. Because the chance of that happening is so rare we would reject the null hypothesis. We would say “guilty!” 05
  • 94. Because the chance of that happening is so rare we would reject the null hypothesis. We would say “guilty!” But if in reality there is no difference, 05
  • 95. Because the chance of that happening is so rare we would reject the null hypothesis. We would say “guilty!” But if in reality there is no difference, then we have made a type I error. 05
  • 96. Because the chance of that happening is so rare we would reject the null hypothesis. We would say “guilty!” But if in reality there is no difference, then we have made a type I error. 05 Researchers are willing to take that chance.
  • 97. In conclusion, hypothesis testing, is a way of determining the probability of our default position (not guilty or no difference) being correct or incorrect.
  • 98. In conclusion, hypothesis testing, is a way of determining the probability of our default position (not guilty or no difference) being correct or incorrect. We determine the likelihood of being right or wrong based on the results.
  • 99. In conclusion, hypothesis testing, is a way of determining the probability of our default position (not guilty or no difference) being correct or incorrect. We determine the likelihood of being right or wrong based on the results. Then we decide if we are willing to maintain our default position (no difference) or go out on a limb and change our default position (yes there is a difference).
  • 100. What follows are exercises to help you check your understanding.
  • 101. Go as far as you feel you need to until you have a good feel for what you know.
  • 102. First Set of Questions
  • 103. 1. Which expression below from the world of judicial decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”?
  • 104. 1. Which expression below from the world of judicial decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? A. “Guilty as charged” B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” C. “Pleading no contest”
  • 105. 1. Which expression below from the world of judicial decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? A. “Guilty as charged” B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” C. “Pleading no contest”
  • 106. 1. Which expression below from the world of judicial decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? A. “Guilty as charged” B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” C. “Pleading no contest” 2. What is another way to say “Null-hypothesis”?
  • 107. 1. Which expression below from the world of judicial decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? A. “Guilty as charged” B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” C. “Pleading no contest” 2. What is another way to say “Null-hypothesis”? A. Not clear B. Not different C. Not important
  • 108. 1. Which expression below from the world of judicial decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? A. “Guilty as charged” B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” C. “Pleading no contest” 2. What is another way to say “Null-hypothesis”? A. Not clear B. Not different C. Not important
  • 109. With hypothesis testing we are attempting to set up a default position of not guilty. We stay in that position unless we have enough evidence to overturn it.
  • 110. With hypothesis testing we are attempting to set up a default position of not guilty. We stay in that position unless we have enough evidence to overturn it. Let’s say our null-hypothesis is the following:
  • 111. With hypothesis testing we are attempting to set up a default position of not guilty. We stay in that position unless we have enough evidence to overturn it. Let’s say our null-hypothesis is the following: There is no difference in IQ between children who are exposed to classical music between the ages of 0 and 3 and those who were not.
  • 112. With hypothesis testing we are attempting to set up a default position of not guilty. We stay in that position unless we have enough evidence to overturn it. Let’s say our null-hypothesis is the following: There is no difference in IQ between children who are exposed to classical music between the ages of 0 and 3 and those who were not. This is our default position. We are not neutral, we are claiming at the outset that there is no difference.
  • 113. But then along comes some evidence that over turns that position. So we reject the null hypothesis and claim there is a probable difference.
  • 114. But then along comes some evidence that over turns that position. So we reject the null hypothesis and claim there is a probable difference. Notice how we don’t say “there is a difference”. We say there is a probable or statistical difference. This just means that with statistics we are never 100% certain. We just say that the probability that we are wrong is a certain percent. Usually that percent needs to be pretty low.
  • 115. If we have estimated that there is a 60% chance that we are wrong, that is a risk not worth taking. If you were told that you had a 60% chance of losing a lot of money and a 40% chance of making a lot of money, would you take that chance? Probably not. But if you were told that you had only a 5% chance of losing a lot of money and a 95% of earning a lot, that might be a chance you would be willing to take. The same holds true with hypothesis testing.
  • 116. If we have estimated that there is a 60% chance that we are wrong, that is a risk not worth taking. If you were told that you had a 60% chance of losing a lot of money and a 40% chance of making a lot of money, would you take that chance? Probably not. But if you were told that you had only a 5% chance of losing a lot of money and a 95% of earning a lot, that might be a chance you would be willing to take. The same holds true with hypothesis testing.
  • 117. Based on that instruction, consider your answer to these questions again and explain the correct answer in your own words.
  • 118. Based on that instruction, consider your answer to these questions again and explain the correct answer in your own words. 1. Which expression below from the world of judicial decision-making best describes the “Null-hypothesis”? A. “Guilty as charged” B. “Not guilty until proven innocent” C. “Pleading no contest” 2. What is another way to say “Null-hypothesis”? A. Not clear B. Not different C. Not important
  • 119. Second Set of Questions – see if you can answer these questions, if not go to the instruction that follows and you’ll be given an opportunity to respond to the questions armed with the instruction.
  • 120. 3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so.
  • 121. 3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so.
  • 122. 3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so.
  • 123. 3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so.
  • 124. 3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. 4. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant actually was “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so.
  • 125. 3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. 4. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant actually was “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so.
  • 126. 3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. 4. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant actually was “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so.
  • 127. 3. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant really is “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. 4. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant actually was “not guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so.
  • 128. 5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so.
  • 129. 5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so.
  • 130. 5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so.
  • 131. 5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so.
  • 132. 5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. 6. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant really is “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so.
  • 133. 5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. 6. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant really is “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so.
  • 134. 5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. 6. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant really is “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so.
  • 135. 5. When the jury decides “not guilty” and the defendant actually was “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were wrong to do so. 6. When the jury decides “guilty” and the defendant really is “guilty”, in statistics that is the same as saying: A. ACCEPT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so. B. REJECT the null hypothesis and it turns out - - - you were right to do so.
  • 136. Accepting the null-hypothesis is essentially like saying “not guilty” or that we accept the default position of innocence or no difference. Rejecting the null-hypothesis is essentially like saying “guilty” or that we reject the default position of innocence or there is enough evidence to suggest there is a difference.
  • 137. Here is a visual:
  • 138. Here is a visual: Null-hypothesis ACCEPTED!
  • 139. Here is a visual: Null-hypothesis ACCEPTED! I was found NOT GUILTY!
  • 140. Here is a visual: Null-hypothesis ACCEPTED! I was found NOT GUILTY! Na, na, . . . nanana! There is NOT enough statistical evidence to convict or reject the null-hypothesis!
  • 141. Here is a visual: Null-hypothesis ACCEPTED! I was found NOT GUILTY! Na, na, . . . nanana! There is NOT enough statistical evidence to convict or reject the null-hypothesis! Not Guilty = Accept the Null
  • 142. Here is a visual: Null-hypothesis REJECTED!
  • 143. Here is a visual: Null-hypothesis REJECTED! I was found GUILTY!
  • 144. Here is a visual: Null-hypothesis REJECTED! I was found GUILTY! Wa, Wa! There IS enough statistical evidence to convict or reject the null- Hypothesis!
  • 145. Here is a visual: Null-hypothesis REJECTED! I was found GUILTY! Wa, Wa! There IS enough statistical evidence to convict or reject the null- Hypothesis! Guilty = Reject the Null
  • 146. Third Set of Questions - see if you can answer these questions, if not go to the instruction that follows and you’ll be given an opportunity to respond to the questions armed with the instruction.
  • 147. 7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 148. 7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error 8. When the jury decides “not guilty” (accept the null) and the defendant actually was “guilty” (reject the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 149. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 150. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error 10. Question: What is the haunting reality? Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is determining the probability that we . . . have committed an error. are correct in our hypothesis.
  • 151. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error 10. Question: What is the haunting reality? Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is determining the probability that we . . . have committed an error. are correct in our hypothesis.
  • 152. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error 10. Question: What is the haunting reality? Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is determining the probability that we . . . A. have committed an error. B. are correct in our hypothesis.
  • 153. Let’s consider each type of error
  • 154. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence, 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis, 4. You accept the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have rejected the null. 6. This is a type I error
  • 155. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis, 4. You accept the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have rejected the null. 6. This is a type I error
  • 156. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You accept the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have rejected the null. 6. This is a type I error
  • 157. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You accept the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have rejected the null. 6. This is a type I error
  • 158. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You accept the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have rejected the null. 6. This is a type I error
  • 159. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You accept the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is a difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have rejected the null. This is a type I error
  • 160. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You reject the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have accepted the null This is a type II error
  • 161. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You reject the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have accepted the null This is a type II error
  • 162. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You reject the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have accepted the null This is a type II error
  • 163. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You reject the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have accepted the null This is a type II error
  • 164. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You reject the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have accepted the null This is a type II error
  • 165. 1. State your null-hypothesis; There is no significant difference between females and males in terms of their preference of certain sports-car colors 2. Collect your evidence 3. Determine if the evidence merits accepting or rejecting the null-hypothesis 4. You reject the null 5. In reality (and you could never know this for sure) you were wrong. In actuality there is NO difference between men and women sports-car color preference and you should have accepted the null This is a type II error
  • 166. You’ll never know if you committed a type I or II error. You can only estimate the probability that you did!
  • 167. That’s because with statistics we deal in probability, not certainty.
  • 168. Based on the instruction you just received, respond to these questions again. Explain your reasoning for selecting the options you did.
  • 169. 7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 170. 7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 171. 7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error 8. When the jury decides “not guilty” (accept the null) and the defendant actually was “guilty” (reject the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 172. 7. When the jury decides “guilty” (reject the null) and the defendant actually was “not guilty” (shouldn’t have rejected the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error 8. When the jury decides “not guilty” (accept the null) and the defendant actually was “guilty” (reject the null), what type of error has been committed? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 173. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 174. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error
  • 175. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error 10. Question: What is the haunting reality? Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is determining the probability that we . . . have committed an error. are correct in our hypothesis.
  • 176. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error 10. Question: What is the haunting reality? Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is determining the probability that we . . . have committed an error. are correct in our hypothesis.
  • 177. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error 10. Question: What is the haunting reality? Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is determining the probability that we . . . A. have committed an error. B. are correct in our hypothesis. Answers: 7-A, 8-B, 9-B, 10-A
  • 178. 9. Which type of error is preferable? A. Type I error B. Type II error 10. Question: What is the haunting reality? Answer: We actually never know for sure if we have committed a type I or II error. All we are doing is determining the probability that we . . . A. have committed an error. B. are correct in our hypothesis. Answers: 7-A, 8-B, 9-B, 10-A
  • 179. Fourth Set of Questions - see if you can answer these questions, if not go to the instruction that follows and you’ll be given an opportunity to respond to the questions armed with the instruction.
  • 180. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong
  • 181. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong
  • 182. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong 12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 times from a population, we would be willing to _____________________ .05 or 5% of the time a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . .
  • 183. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong 12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 times from a population, we would be willing to _____________________ .05 or 5% of the time a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . .
  • 184. In statistics we generally ask ourselves, “What is the probability that we have made a type I error?”
  • 185. In statistics we generally ask ourselves, “What is the probability that we have made a Type I Error?” Type I errors are considered a bigger issue because if we are wrong, than we might waste a lot of money or impact people negatively (e.g., spend millions of dollars on a new drug that doesn’t work).
  • 186. In statistics we generally ask ourselves, “What is the probability that we have made a Type I Error?” Type I errors are considered a bigger issue because if we are wrong, than we might waste a lot of money or impact people negatively (e.g., spend millions of dollars on a new drug that doesn’t work). Type II errors are considered less of an issue because if we are wrong, than we may stop or continue researching.
  • 187. We have to have determine a cut-off point as to when we will reject the null-hypothesis. No matter what cut-off point we could have chosen, the decision would always have been somewhat arbitrary.
  • 188. We have to have determine a cut-off point as to when we will reject the null-hypothesis. No matter what cut-off point we could have chosen, the decision would always have been somewhat arbitrary. Would we be satisfied with a 75% chance of committing a type I error? Probably not. That means out of 100 experiments we would live with being wrong about our conclusions 75 times.
  • 189. Would we be satisfied with a .01% chance of committing a type I error? Probably not. That means out of 10,000 experiments we would live with being wrong about our conclusions only once. If that were the case, then almost no null-hypothesis could ever be rejected.
  • 190. Would we be satisfied with a .01% chance of committing a type I error? Probably not. That means out of 10,000 experiments we would live with being wrong about our conclusions only once. If that were the case, then almost no null-hypothesis could ever be rejected. In the discipline of statistics .05 or 5% of a chance of committing a type I error has been deemed an acceptable arbitrary cut-off point. This means that out of 100 experiments we will live with being wrong five times.
  • 191. Based on the instruction you just received, respond to these questions again. Explain your reasoning for selecting the options you did.
  • 192. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong
  • 193. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong
  • 194. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong
  • 195. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong 12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 times from a population, we would be willing to _____________________ .05 or 5% of the time a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . .
  • 196. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong 12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 times from a population, we would be willing to _____________________ .05 or 5% of the time a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . . Answers: 11-B, 12-A
  • 197. 11. Question: How do we decide how much evidence is required before we will reject the null hypothesis? Answer: We estimate the probability of being ______ a certain percent of the time (e.g., .05 or 5% of the time). a. right b. wrong 12. Question: What does a .05 rejection level mean? Answer: If we were to take the same small sample 100 times from a population, we would be willing to _____________________ .05 or 5% of the time a. . . . take the chance of being wrong . . . b. . . . reject the null hypothesis . . . Answers: 11-B, 12-A